Immediate and Distracted Imitation in Second-Language Speech: Unreleased Plosives In English
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.2478/v10015-012-0007-7Keywords:
imitation, plosives, unreleased, distractionAbstract
The paper investigates immediate and distracted imitation in second-language speech using unreleased plosives. Unreleased plosives are fairly frequently found in English sequences of two stops. Polish, on the other hand, is characterised by a significant rate of releases in such sequences. This cross-linguistic difference served as material to look into how and to what extent non-native properties of sounds can be produced in immediate and distracted imitation. Thirteen native speakers of Polish first read and then imitated sequences of words with two stops straddling the word boundary. Stimuli for imitation had no release of the first stop. The results revealed that (1) a non-native feature such as the lack of the release burst can be imitated; (2) distracting imitation impedes imitative performance; (3) the type of a sequence interacts with the magnitude of an imitative effect
References
Abercrombie, D. 1967. Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 28: 105-167.
Google Scholar
Babel, M. 2010. Dialect convergence and divergence in New Zealand English. Language in Society 39: 437-456.
Google Scholar
Babel, M. 2012. Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. Journal of Phonetics 40: 177-189.
Google Scholar
Bałutowa, B. 1974. Wymowa Angielska dla Wszystkich. Warszawa: Wiedza Powszechna.
Google Scholar
Bergier, M. 2010. The occurrence of unreleased oral stops in English voice agreeing plosive clusters straddling word boundaries. Production experiments with Polish advanced learners of English. M. A. Thesis. University of Silesia.
Google Scholar
Best, C. T. and W. Strange. 1992. Effects of language-specific phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics 20: 305-330.
Google Scholar
Boersma, P. 2001. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International 10: 341-345.
Google Scholar
Bourhis, R. Y. and H. Giles. 1977. The language of intergroup distinctiveness. In H. Giles (ed.) Language, Ethnicity, and Intergroup Relations. London: Academic Press: 119-136.
Google Scholar
Byrd, D. 1992. Preliminary results on speaker-dependent variation in the TIMIT database. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 92: 593-596.
Google Scholar
Byrd, D. 1993. 54,000 American stops. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 83: 1-19.
Google Scholar
Byrne, D. 1971. The Attraction Paradigm. New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Chambers, J. 1992. Dialect acquisition. Language 68: 673-705.
Google Scholar
Coleman, J. 2002. Phonetic representations in the mental lexicon. In J. Durand and B. Laks (eds) Phonetics, Phonology and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 96-130.
Google Scholar
Crystal, T. H. and A. S. House. 1988a. The duration of American-English stop consonants. Journal of Phonetics 16: 285-294.
Google Scholar
Crystal, T. H. and A. S. House. 1988b Segmental durations in connected-speech signals: Current results. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 83: 1553-1573.
Google Scholar
Davidson, L. 2010. Variation in stop releases in American English spontaneous speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 128: 2458.
Google Scholar
Delvaux, V. and A. Soquet. 2007. The influence of ambient speech on adult speech productions through unintentional imitation. Phonetica 64: 145-173.
Google Scholar
Dukiewicz, L. and I. Sawicka. 1995. Fonetyka i fonologia. In H. Wróbel (ed.) Gramatyka i Fonologia Języka Polskiego. Kraków: Instytut Języka Polskiego PAN.
Google Scholar
Evans, B. G. and P. Iverson. 2007. Plasticity in vowel perception and production: A study of accent change in young adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121: 3814-3826.
Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A., J. M. Brown, L. Sabadini and J. Weihing. 2003. Rapid access to speech gestures in perception: Evidence from choice and simple response time tasks. Journal of Memory and Language 49: 396-413.
Google Scholar
Gentilucci, B. R. Bernardis. 2007. Imitation during phoneme production. Neuropsychologia 45: 608-615.
Google Scholar
Gentilucci, B. and M. C. Corballis. 2006. From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews 30: 949-960.
Google Scholar
Giles, H., J. Coupland and N. Coupland. 1991. Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Gimson, A. C. 2001. Gimson's Pronunciation of English. 6th edition. Rev. by A. Cruttenden. London: Arnold.
Google Scholar
Goldinger, S. D. 1997. Perception and production in an episodic lexicon. In K. Johnson and J. W. Mullennix (eds) Talker Variability in Speech Processing. San Diego: Academic Press: 33-66.
Google Scholar
Goldinger, S. D. 1998. Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review 105: 251-279.
Google Scholar
Gregory, S. W. 1990. Analysis of fundamental frequency reveals covariation in interview partners’ speech. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 14: 237-251.
Google Scholar
Gregory, S. W., K. Dagan and S. Webster. 1997. Evaluating the relation between vocal accommodation in conversational partners’ fundamental frequencies to perceptions of communication quality. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 21: 23-43.
Google Scholar
Gregory, S. W., B. E. Green, R. M. Carrothers and K. A. Dagan. 2001. Verifying the primacy of voice fundamental frequency in social status accommodation. Language Communication 21: 37-60.
Google Scholar
Gregory, S. W. and S. Webster. 1996. A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70: 1231-1240.
Google Scholar
Gregory, S. W., S. Webster and G. Huang. 1993. Voice pitch and amplitude convergence as a metric of quality in dyadic interviews. Language Communication 13: 195-217.
Google Scholar
Halle, P. A., C. T. Best and A. Levitt. 1999. Phonetic vs. phonological influences on French listeners' perception of American English approximants. Journal of Phonetics 27: 281-306.
Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. 1996. The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Henderson, J. B. and B. H. Repp. 1982. Is a stop consonant released when followed by another stop consonant? Phonetica 39: 71-82.
Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. 1986. "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 93: 411-428.
Google Scholar
Honorof, D. N., J. Weihing and C. A. Fowler. 2011. Articulatory events are imitated under rapid shadowing. Journal of Phonetics 39: 18-38.
Google Scholar
Horner, V. and A. Whiten. 2005. Casual knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition 8: 164-181.
Google Scholar
Jamieson, D. G. and D. E. Moroson. 1986. Training non-native speech contrasts in adults: Acquisition of the English /ð/ - /Ɵ/ contrast by francophones. Perception and Psychophysics 40: 205-215.
Google Scholar
Jassem, W. 1974. Fonetyka Języka Angielskiego. 4th edition. Warszawa: PWN.
Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. J. and G. H. Yeni-Komshian. 1995. Cross-language speech perception: Perspective and promise. In W. Strange (ed.) Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Speech Research. York: York Press: 463-479.
Google Scholar
Johnson, K. 1997. Speech perception without speaker normalization: An exemplar model. In K. Johnson and J. W. Mullennix (eds) Talker Variability in Speech Processing. San Diego: Academic Press: 145-166.
Google Scholar
Jones, D. 1956. An Outline of English Phonetics. Cambridge: Heffer and Sons.
Google Scholar
Kondaurova, M. V. and A. L. Francis. 2008. The relationship between native allophonic experience with vowel duration and perception of the English tense/lax vowel contrast by Spanish and Russian listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124: 3959-3971.
Google Scholar
Kopczyński, A. 1977. Polish and American English Consonant Phonemes. Warszawa: PWN.
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. and A. N. Meltzoff. 1996. Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100: 2425-2438.
Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. 1975. A Course in Phonetics. Orlando: Harcourt Brace.
Google Scholar
Mańkowska, A., M. Nowacka and M. Kłoczowska. 2009. "How Much Wood Would a Woodchuck Chuck?": English Pronunciation Practice Book. Kraków: Wydawnictwo WSE.
Google Scholar
McAllister, R., F. E. Flege and T. Piske. 2002. The influence of L1 on the acquisition of Swedish quantity by native speakers of Spanish, English and Estonian. Journal of Phonetics 30: 229-258.
Google Scholar
McClaskey, C. L., D. B. Pisoni and T. D. Carrell. 1983. Transfer of training of a new linguistic contrast in voicing. Perception and Psychophysics 34: 323-330.
Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. and M. Moore. 1999. Persons and representation: Why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In J. Nadel and G. Butterworth (eds) Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 9-35.
Google Scholar
Mitterer, H. and M. Ernestus. 2008. The link between speech perception and production is phonological and abstract: Evidence form the shadowing task. Cognition 109: 168- 173.
Google Scholar
Munro, M. J., T. M. Derwing, J. E. Flege. 1999. Canadians in Alabama: A perceptual study of dialect acquisition in adults. Journal of Phonetics 27: 385-403.
Google Scholar
Nagell, K., K. Olguin and M. Tomasello. 1993. Processes of social learning in tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107: 174-186.
Google Scholar
Namy, L. L., L. C. Nygaard and D. Sauerteig. 2002. Gender differences in vocal accommodation: The role of perception. Journal and Language and Social Psychology 21: 422-432.
Google Scholar
Natale, M. 1975. Convergence of mean vocal intensity in dyadic communication as a function of social desirability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32: 790- 804.
Google Scholar
Nielsen, K. 2011. Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics 39: 132-14.
Google Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. 1986. Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115: 39-57.
Google Scholar
Pardo, J. S. 2006. On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: 2382-2392.
Google Scholar
Pardo, J. S. 2010. Expressing oneself in conversational interaction. In E. Morsella (ed) Expressing Oneself/Expressing One’s Self. Communication, Cognition, Language, and Identity. New York: Psychology Press: 183-196.
Google Scholar
Pardo, J. S., I. Cajori Jay and R. M. Krauss. 2010. Conversational role influences speech imitation. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 72: 2254-2264.
Google Scholar
Pardo, J. S., R. Gibbons, A. Suppes and R. M. Krauss. 2012. Phonetic convergence in college roommates. Journal of Phonetics 40: 190-197.
Google Scholar
Pekkola, J., M. Laasonen, V. Ojanen, T. Autti, L. P. Jaaskelainen, T. Kujala et al. 2006. Perception of matching and conflicting audiovisual speech in dyslexic and fluent readers: An fMRI study at 3T. NeuroImage 29: 797-807.
Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. B. 2006. The next toolkit. Journal of Phonetics 34: 516-530.
Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., R. N. Aslin, A. J. Perey and B. L. Hennessy. 1982. Some effects of laboratory training on identification and discrimination of voicing contrasts in stop consonants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8: 297-314.
Google Scholar
Pruitt, P. and J. van Santen. 2006. Training the perception of Hindi dental and retroflex stops by native speakers of English and Japanese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: 1684-1696.
Google Scholar
Pulvermuller, F., M. Huss, F. Kherif, F. Moscoso del Prado Martin, O. Hauk and Y. Shtyrov. 2006. Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103: 7865-7870.
Google Scholar
Randolph, M. 1989. Syllable-based Constraints on Properties of English Sounds. Doctoral Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and M. A. Arbib. 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21: 188-194.
Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and L. Craighero. 2004. The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 21: 169-192.
Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi and V. Gallese. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 661-670.
Google Scholar
Roach, P. English Phonetics and Phonology. 3rd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Rojczyk, A. 2008. Release burst in Polish homorganic stop geminates. Linguistica Silesiana 29: 75-86.
Google Scholar
Rojczyk, A. 2012a. Spontaneous phonetic imitation of L2 vowels in a rapid shadowing task. Paper presented at Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching 2012. Vancouver Canada, August 24-25 2012.
Google Scholar
Rojczyk, A. 2012b. Phonetic and phonological mode in second-language speech: VOT imitation. Paper presented at EUROSLA 2012, Poznań Poland, September 5-8 2012.
Google Scholar
Schwartz, J-L., A. Basirat, L. Ménard and M. Sato. 2012. The Perception-for-Action- Control Theory (PACT): A perceptuo-motor theory of speech perception. Journal of Neurolinguistics 25: 336-354.
Google Scholar
Shepard, C. A., H. Giles and B. A. Le Poire. 2001. Communication accommodation theory. In W. P. Robinson and H. Giles (eds) The New Handbook of Language and Social Psychology. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Google Scholar
Shockley, K., L. Sabadini and C. A. Fowler. 2004. Imitation in shadowing words. Perception and Psychophysics 66: 422-429.
Google Scholar
Skipper, J. I., V. Van Wassenhove, H. C. Nusbaum and S. L. Small. 2007. Hearing lips and seeing voices: How cortical areas supporting speech production mediate audiovisual speech perception. Cerebral Cortex 17: 2387-2399.
Google Scholar
Sobkowiak, W. 2001. English Phonetics for Poles. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Poznańskie.
Google Scholar
Street, R. L. and H. Giles. 1982. Speech accommodation theory: A social cognitive approach to language and speech behavior. In M. Roloff and C. Berger (eds) Social Cognition and Communication. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. and L. M. Triandis. 1960. Race, social class, religion, and nationality as determinants of social distance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 61: 110- 118.
Google Scholar
Trudgill, P. 1986. Dialects in Contact. New York: Blackwell Publishing.
Google Scholar
Whiten, A. and D. M. Custance. 1996. Studies of imitation in chimpanzees and children. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef (eds) Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego: Academic Press: 291-318.
Google Scholar
Wierzchowska, B. 1980. Fonetyka i Fonologia Języka Polskiego. Wrocław: PAN.
Google Scholar
Wilson, S. M. and M. Iacoboni. 2006. Neural responses to non-native phonemes varying in producibility: Evidence for the sensimotor nature of speech perception. NeuroImage 33: 316-325.
Google Scholar
Wilson, S. M., A. P. Saygin, M. I. Sereno and M. Iacoboni. 2004. Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience 7: 701- 702.
Google Scholar
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.