Thirty years of social neuroscience
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18778/1427-969X.23.01Keywords:
social neuroscience, cognitive neuroscience, loneliness, social statusAbstract
Over the last three decades, social neuroscience, which had emerged as a subdiscipline of cognitive neuroscience at the beginning of 1990s, has been rapidly developing its specific methodology and extending its research scope. By combining knowledge and methodology from social, psychological and biological sciences it is uniquely suited to study human functioning on multiple levels and elucidate the trajectories underlying human social behavior. The current article aims to briefly cover the history of social neuroscience by discussing development of both research and academic structures related to it. Moreover, the research on the association between social isolation and subjective social status, and health is presented as an example of practical application of the interdisciplinary approach adopted by social neuroscience.
References
Baron-Cohen S.E., Tager-Flusberg H.E., Cohen D.J. (1994). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Bolis D., Schilbach L. (2018). Observing and participating in social interactions: Action perception and action control across the autistic spectrum. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 29, 168–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.009
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.009
Breuning L.G. (2021). Status games: Why we play and how to stop. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.
Google Scholar
Brothers L. (1990). The neural basis of primate social communication. Motivation and Emotion, 14(2), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00991637
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00991637
Cacioppo J.T., Berntson G.G. (1992). Social psychological contributions to the decade of the brain: Doctrine of multilevel analysis. American Psychologist, 47(8), 1019. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.8.1019
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.8.1019
Cacioppo J.T., Cacioppo S. (2018). Loneliness in the modern age: An evolutionary theory of loneliness (ETL). W: J. Olson (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 58, 127–197). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.03.003
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.03.003
Cacioppo J.T., Decety J. (2011). An introduction to social neuroscience. W: J.T. Cacioppo, J. Decety (eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (3–8). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342161.001.0001
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342161.001.0001
Cacioppo J.T., Hawkley L.C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005
Cacioppo J.T., Patrick W. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Google Scholar
Campagne D.M. (2019). Stress and perceived social isolation (loneliness). Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 82, 192–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2019.02.007
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2019.02.007
Caplan G. (1974). Support systems and community mental health: Lectures on concept development. Behavioral publications.
Google Scholar
Decety J., Keenan J.P. (2006). Social neuroscience: A new journal. Social Neuroscience, 1(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600683549
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600683549
Dunbar R.I. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–694. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325
Dunbar R.I. (1995). Neocortex size and group size in primates: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 28(3), 287–296. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1995.1021
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1995.1021
Dunbar R.I. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 6(5), 178–190. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8
Ekman P., Levenson R., Friesen W. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221(4616), 1208–1210. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338
Ernst J.M., Cacioppo J.T. (1999). Lonely hearts: Psychological perspectives on loneliness. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 8(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80008-0
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80008-0
Euteneuer F. (2014). Subjective social status and health. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 27(5), 337–343. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000083
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000083
Frith U. (1994). Autism and theory of mind in everyday life. Social Development, 3(2), 108–124. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9507.1994.tb00031.x
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1994.tb00031.x
Gallese V., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Rizzolatti G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593
Hari R., Henriksson L., Malinen S., Parkkonen L. (2015). Centrality of social interaction in human brain function. Neuron, 88(1), 181–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.022
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.022
Harlow J.M. (1868). Recovery after severe injury to the head. Publication of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2(327), 990–992.
Google Scholar
Hawkley L.C., Capitanio J.P. (2015). Perceived social isolation, evolutionary fitness and health outcomes: A lifespan approach. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1669), 20140114. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0114
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0114
Haxby J.V., Gobbini M.I., Furey M.L., Ishai A., Schouten J.L., Pietrini P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293(5539), 2425–2430. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1063736
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1063736
Hertz N. (2020). The lonely century: Coming together in a world that’s pulling apart. London: Sceptre.
Google Scholar
Hoebel J., Maske U.E., Zeeb H., Lampert T. (2017). Social inequalities and depressive symptoms in adults: The role of objective and subjective socioeconomic status. PloS one, 12(1), e0169764. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169764
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169764
Holt-Lunstad J., Smith T.B., Baker M., Harris T., Stephenson D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352
Holwerda T.J., Beekman A.T., Deeg D.J., Stek M.L., van Tilburg T.G., Visser P.J., Schmands B., Jonker C., Schoevers R.A. (2012). Increased risk of mortality associated with social isolation in older men: Only when feeling lonely? Results from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL). Psychological Medicine, 42(4), 843–853. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001772
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001772
Hooker E.D., Campos B., Zoccola P.M., Dickerson S.S. (2018). Subjective socioeconomic status matters less when perceived social support is high: A study of cortisol responses to stress. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(8), 981–989. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617732387
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617732387
Jaśkowski P. (2009). Neuronauka poznawcza: Jak mózg tworzy umysł. Warszawa: Vizja Press & IT.
Google Scholar
Johansson G. (1978). Visual event perception. W: R. Held, H.W. Leibowitz, H.L. Teubner (eds.), Perception (675–711). Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-46354-9_22
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-46354-9_22
Kanwisher N., McDermott J., Chun M.M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997
Kilner J.M., Lemon R.N. (2013). What we know currently about mirror neurons. Current Biology, 23(23), R1057–R1062.
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.051
Lam J.A., Murray E.R., Yu K.E., Ramsey M., Nguyen T.T., Mishra J., Martis B., Thomas M.L., Lee E.E. (2021). Neurobiology of loneliness: A systematic review. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(11), 1873–1887. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01058-7
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01058-7
Lieberman M.D. (2006). Social cognitive and affective neuroscience: When opposites attract. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(1), 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl010
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl010
Lieberman M.D. (2012). A geographical history of social cognitive neuroscience. Neuroimage, 61(2), 432–436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.089
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.089
Marmot M. (2007). The status syndrome: How social standing affects our health and longevity. New York: Macmillan Publishers.
Google Scholar
Marmot M.G., Rose G., Shipley M., Hamilton P.J. (1978). Employment grade and coronary heart disease in British civil servants. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 32(4), 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.32.4.244
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.32.4.244
Matusall S., Kaufmann I.M., Christen M. (2011). The emergence of social neuroscience as an academic discipline. W: J.T. Cacioppo, J. Decety (eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (10–27). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342161.013.0002
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342161.013.0002
Miller G., Chen E., Cole S.W. (2009). Health psychology: Developing biologically plausible models linking the social world and physical health. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 501–524. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163551
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163551
Muscatell K.A., Morelli S.A., Falk E.B., Way B.M., Pfeifer J.H., Galinsky A.D., Lieberman M.D., Dapretto M., Eisenberger N.I. (2012). Social status modulates neural activity in the mentalizing network. Neuroimage, 60(3), 1771–1777.
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.080
Myin‐Germeys I., Kasanova Z., Vaessen T., Vachon H., Kirtley O., Viechtbauer W., Reininghaus U. (2018). Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: New insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry, 17(2), 123–132.
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20513
Okruszek Ł., Dolan K., Lawrence M., Cella M. (2017). The beat of social cognition: Exploring the role of heart rate variability as marker of mentalizing abilities. Social Neuroscience, 12(5), 489–493. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1244113
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1244113
Okruszek Ł., Jarkiewicz M., Gola M., Cella M., Łojek E. (2018a). Using ERPs to explore the impact of affective distraction on working memory stages in schizophrenia. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(3), 437–446. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0578-4
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0578-4
Okruszek Ł., Piejka A., Wysokiński A., Szczepocka E., Manera V. (2018b). Biological motion sensitivity, but not interpersonal predictive coding is impaired in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(3), 305. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000335
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000335
Okruszek Ł., Jarkiewicz M., Szrubarz P., Wichniak A., Michałowski J., Marchewka A., Łojek E. (2019). Many ways to forget: Neurophysiology of directed forgetting mechanisms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 274, 358–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.02.057
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.02.057
Okruszek Ł., Piejka A., Krawczyk M., Schudy A., Wiśniewska M., Żurek K., Pinkham A. (2021). Owner of a lonely mind? Social cognitive capacity is associated with objective, but not perceived social isolation in healthy individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 93, 104103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104103
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104103
Onnela J.P. (2021). Opportunities and challenges in the collection and analysis of digital phenotyping data. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 45–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0771-3
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0771-3
Payne K. (2018). The broken ladder: How inequality affects the way we think, live, and die. London: Penguin Books.
Google Scholar
Piejka A., Wiśniewska M., Thayer J.F., Okruszek Ł. (2021). Brief induction of loneliness decreases vagal regulation during social information processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 164, 112–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.03.002
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.03.002
Pieritz K., Süssenbach P., Rief W., Euteneuer F. (2016). Subjective social status and cardiovascular reactivity: An experimental examination. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1091. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01091
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01091
Posner M.I., DiGirolamo G.J. (2000). Cognitive neuroscience: Origins and promise. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.873
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.873
Singer T. (2012). The past, present and future of social neuroscience: An European perspective. Neuroimage, 61(2), 437–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.109
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.109
Stanley D.A., Adolphs R. (2013). Toward a neural basis for social behavior. Neuron, 80(3), 816–826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.038
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.038
Tajfel H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 23(5), 96–102. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1170-96
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1170-96
Thayer J.F., Lane R.D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61(3), 201–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4
Valtorta N.K., Kanaan M., Gilbody S., Ronzi S., Hanratty B. (2016). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: Systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart, 102(13), 1009–1016. https://doi.org/10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790
Google Scholar
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790
Winkielman P. (2008). Psychologia społeczna a neuronauki: Dominacja, separacja czy satysfakcjonujący związek. Psychologia Społeczna, 3(1), 6.
Google Scholar
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.