Język jako narzędzie poznania i komunikacji
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18778/1427-969X.17.02Słowa kluczowe:
language, cognition, linguistic categories, stereotypesAbstrakt
The paper presents arguments for treating language as a tool for cognition and communications. It articulates an opposite view to the one which considers language as an independent module shaping cognition and thinking. According to the conception of the socially – based cognition the mind is a controller of the adaptive behavior, and communication is a strategic action to which language is subjected. The model of the linguistic categories arranges words according to the level of their abstraction; and a number of research results presented in the paper indicate that there is a relationship between the level of abstraction of the words used and the inference related to events, emotions and memory. Recognizing language as a tool for cognition and communication leads to the acceptance of the necessity to widen the language awareness. This kind of thinking is supported by the presented results of research on the relationship between linguistic categories and the stereotypes, communication of the interpersonal distance and the process of asking questions and giving answers.
Bibliografia
Adamska, K. (2012). Iluzja transparentności. Przyczyny i skutki. Studia Psychologiczne, 4, 13–25.
Google Scholar
Aron, A., Aron, E.N., Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612.
Google Scholar
Beukeboom, C.J., Semin, R.G. (2005). Mood and representation of behaviour: The how and why. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1242–1251.
Google Scholar
Brown, R., Fish, D. (1983). The psychological causality implicit in language. Cognition, 14, 237–273.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2000). On nature of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Coenen, L.H.M., Hedebouw, L., Semin, G.R. (2006). The Linguistic Category Model (LCM). Manual. Free University Amsterdam.
Google Scholar
DeCoster, J., Banner, M.J., Smith, E.R., Semin, G.R. (2006). On the inexplicability of the implicit: differences in the information provided by implicit and explicit tests. Social Cognition, 24, 5–21.
Google Scholar
Douglas, K.M., Sutton, R.M. (2010). By their words ye shall know them: language abstraction and the likeability of describers. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 366–374.
Google Scholar
Grabias, S. (2012). Mowa i jej zaburzenia. [W:] S. Grabias, M. Kurkowski (red.), Logopedia. Teoria zaburzeń mowy (s.15–91). Lublin: Wydawnictwo UMCS.
Google Scholar
Grice, P. (1980). Logika a konwersacja. [W:] Język w świetle nauki, (przeł.) B. Stanosz (s. 91–114).Warszawa: Czytelnik.
Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1999). Teoria działania komunikacyjnego, t. 1: Racjonalność działania a racjonalność społeczna. Warszawa: PWN.
Google Scholar
IJzerman, H., Semin, G.R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations: mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20, 1214–1220.
Google Scholar
Jiga-Boy, G.M., Clark, A.E., Semin, G.R. (2013). Situating construal level: the function of abstractness and concreteness in social context. Social Cognition, 31, 201–221.
Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Matsumoto, H. (1995). Culture, self, and emotion: A cultural perspective on ‘self-conscious’ emotions. [W:] J.P. Tangney, K.W. Fischer (red.), Selfconscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (s. 439–465). New York: Guilford Press.
Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., Larsen, J.T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures. Psychological Science, 14, 201–207.
Google Scholar
Krauss, R.M., Fussel, S.R. (1996). Social psychological models of interpersonal communication. [W:] E.T. Higgins, A.W. Kruglansky (red.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (s.655–701). New York: Guilford.
Google Scholar
Kurcz, I. (2000). Psychologia języka i komunikacji. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar.
Google Scholar
Kurcz, I. (2011). Charakterystyka kompetencji językowej – reprezentacje umysłowe. [W:] I. Kurcz, H. Okuniewska (red.), Język jako przedmiot badań psychologicznych. Psycholingwistyka ogólna i neurolingwistyka. Warszawa: Academica.
Google Scholar
Lee, A.Y., Semin, G.R. (2009). Culture through the Lens of Self-Regulatory Orientations. [W:] R.S. Wyer, C. Chiu, Y. Hong (red.), Understanding Culture: Theory, Research and Application (s.271–288). New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Liberman, N., Trope, J. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 512–526.
Google Scholar
Maass, A., Salvi, C., Arcuri, L., Semin, G.R. (1989). Language use in intergroup contexts: The linguistic intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 981–993.
Google Scholar
Maass, A., Milesi, A., Zabbini, S., Stahlberg, D. (1995). The linguistic intergroup bias: Differential expectancies or in-group-protection? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 116–126.
Google Scholar
Maass, A., Ceccarelli, R., Rudin, S. (1996). Linguistic intergroup bias: Evidence for in-group protective motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 512–526.
Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow & Company.
Google Scholar
Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. (2011). Zjednoczeni w mowie. Względność językowa w ujęciu dynamicznym. Warszawa: Scholar.
Google Scholar
Ross, L., Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. [W:] T. Brown, E.S. Reed, E. Turiel (red.), Values and knowledge (s.103–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R. (1996). The relevance of language for social psychology. [W:] C. McGarty, A. Haslam (red.), The message of social psychology: Perspectives on mind and society (s. 291–304). Oxford: Blackwell.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R. (2000a). Language as a cognitive and behavioral structuring resource: Question – answer exchanges. [W:] W. Stroebe, M. Hewstone (red.), European review of social psychology, Vol. 10 (s. 75–104). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R. (2000b). Agenda 2000: Communication: Language as an implementational device for cognition. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 595–612.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R. (2009). Language, culture, cognition – how do they intersect? [W:] R.S. Wyer, C. Chiu, Y. Hong (red.), Understanding culture: Theory, research and application (s. 259–270). New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Cacioppo, J.T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: synchronization, entrainment, and coordination. [W:] G.R. Semin, E.R. Smith (red.), Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective and neuroscientific approaches (s. 119–147). New York, Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 558–568.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Fiedler, K. (1989). Relocating attributional phenomena within a language-cognition interface: The case of actorsʼ and observersʼperspectives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 491–508.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Fiedler, K. (1991). The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range. [W:] W. Stroebe, M. Hewstone (red.), European Review of Social Psychology, Vol. 2. (s. 1–30). Chichester: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Semin G.R., Fiedler, K. (1992). Language, interaction and social cognition. [W:] G.R. Semin, K. Fiedler (red.), Language, interaction and social cognition (s. 1–10). London, California: Sage Publications.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Garrido, M.V. (2012). A systemic approach to impression formation: from verbal to multimodal process. [W:] J. Forgas, K. Fiedler, C. Sedikides (red.), Social thinking and interpersonal behavior (s. 81–100). New York, Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Görts, C.A., Nandram, S., Semin-Goossenes, A. (2002). Cultural perspectives on the linguistic representation of emotion events. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 11–28.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., De Poot, J.C. (1997). You might regret it if you donʼt notice how a question is worded! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 472–480.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Rubini, M., Fiedler, K. (1995). The answer is in the question: The effect of verb causality upon locus of explanation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 834–842.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Smith, E.R. (1999). Revisiting the past and back to the future: Memory Systems and the Linguistic Representation of Social Events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 877–892.
Google Scholar
Semin, G.R., Smith, E.R. (2013). Socially situated cognition in perspective. Social Cognition, 31, 125–146.
Google Scholar
Schmidt, J. Fiedler, K. (1996). Language and implicit attributions in the Nuremberg trialsʼ Analysing prosecutorsʼ and defence attorneysʼ final speeches. Human Communication Research, 22, 371–398.
Google Scholar
Smith, E.R., DeCoster, J. (1998). Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: Simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 21–35.
Google Scholar
Smith, E.R., Semin, G.R. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition in its social context. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 53–117.
Google Scholar
Smith, P.K., Trope, Y. (2006). You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of the trees: Power priming and abstract information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 578–596.
Google Scholar
Von Hippel, W., Sekaquaptewa, D., Vargas, P. (1997). The Linguistic Intergroup Bias as an implicit indicator of prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 490–509.
Google Scholar
Van Rooijen, M.R., Semin, G.R. van Leeuwen, E. (2007). The effect of linguistic abstraction on interpersonal distance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 817–823.
Google Scholar
Werkman, W.M., Wigboldus, D.H.J., Semin, G.R. (1999). Childrenʼs communication of the linguistic intergroup bias and its impact upon cognitive inferences. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 95–104
Google Scholar
Whorf, B.L. (2002). Język, myśl i rzeczywistość. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo KR.
Google Scholar
Wigboldus, D.H.J., Semin, G.R., Spears, R. (2000). How do we communicate stereotypes? Linguistic bases and inferential consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 5–18.
Google Scholar
Wigboldus, D.H., Semin, G.R., Spears, R. (2006). Communicating expectancies about others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 815–824.
Google Scholar
Williams, L.E., Bargh, J.A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth, promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606–607.
Google Scholar
Wróbel, Sz. (2013). Ewolucja dyspozycji do zachowań kooperacyjnych a komunikacja symboliczna. Przypadek Petera Gärdenforsa. [W:] P. Stalmaszczyk (red.), Metodologie językoznawstwa. Ewolucja języka. Ewolucja teorii językoznawczych (s. 27–53). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego.
Google Scholar
Zwier, S., Semin, G.R. (1996). Tools and tool use in cultural perspective: The case of variation and generality in social cognition. Kurt Lewin Institute: Free University Amsterdam.
Google Scholar
Pobrania
Opublikowane
Jak cytować
Numer
Dział
Licencja
Utwór dostępny jest na licencji Creative Commons Uznanie autorstwa – Użycie niekomercyjne – Bez utworów zależnych 4.0 Międzynarodowe.