Anthropological Review Vol. 86(4), 71–80 (2023)

Anthropological Review

Available online at: https://doi.org/10.18778/1898-6773.86.4.06



Reliability of retrospective assessment of the age of first menstruation


Ryszard Żarów*

Orcidhttps://orcid.org/0000-0001-5006-0798

Department of Anthropology, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University School of Physical Education, 31-571 Krakow, Av. Jana Pawła II 78

Agnieszka Woronkowicz

Orcidhttps://orcid.org/0000-0001-7830-5726

Department of Anthropology, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University School of Physical Education, 31-571 Krakow, Av. Jana Pawła II 78

Barbara Spring

Orcidhttps://orcid.org/0009-0009-9982-2189

Department of Anthropology, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University School of Physical Education, 31-571 Krakow, Av. Jana Pawła II 78

Małgorzata Kowal

Orcidhttps://orcid.org/0000-0003-1548-6536

Department of Anthropology, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University School of Physical Education, 31-571 Krakow, Av. Jana Pawła II 78

Janusz Brudecki

Orcidhttps://orcid.org/0000-0002-3375-7814

Department of Anthropology, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University School of Physical Education, 31-571 Krakow, Av. Jana Pawła II 78




ABSTRACT: The age of the first menstruation is one of the indicators for assessing the course of puberty. It is also a sensitive indicator of the economic situation of individual professional groups or societies, and the low average age of menarche is widely recognized as a marker of society’s well-being. The aim of the study was to analyse the reliability of the retrospective method of assessing the age at menarche by comparing the results to the age obtained from continuous research. Data regarding the age at menarche came from longitudinal somatic development and physical fitness studies conducted between 1976 and 2022. In 2022, 47 women were examined. In continuous studies, the prospective method was used in the assessment of the age of first menstruation while in the 2004 and 2022 studies a retrospective method was applied. Only in 4 out of 47 women the age of the first menstruation declared in 2004 and 2022 (the women were 32–34 and 50–52 years old, respectively) was consistent with the one found in continuous studies. In other cases, there was a discrepancy between the age found in continuous studies and self-reported in 2004 or 2022 or between the age stated in 2004 and 2022. Of those women who were present for the 2022 study, 36 had information about the age of first menstruation from continuous studies and the age of menarche in 2004 was given. For this sample the arithmetic mean and the standard deviation of the age at menarche were calculated. It was found that the retrospective method often used in the assessment of the age of the first menstruation is not fully reliable, as the average discrepancy in the assessment ranged from nearly 1 month (0.05 years) to over 2 months (0.19 years) compared to the prospective method. Women surveyed in 2004 determined the age of the first menstruation more accurately compared to statements obtained 18 years later from the same women. This study suggests that long-term memory (LTM) of a significant life event of every woman is unreliable, as indicated by the difference in the declared age of the first menstruation of women examined in 2004 and 2022, which, in individual cases, was up to 3, 4 or 5 years.

KEY WORDS: menarche, prospective method, retrospective method, continuous research.




Introduction

The age of the first menstruation is one of the indicators for assessing the course of puberty. It is also a sensitive indicator of the economic situation of individual professional groups or societies, and the low average age of menarche is widely recognized as a marker of the well-being of society. It is sometimes included in the analysis of secular trends, acceleration or deceleration of development (Gomuła and Kozieł 2018; Brix et al. 2019; Liu et al. 2021; Pop et al. 2022; Wu et al. 2022 et other). However, regardless of the purpose of the research, the reliability of the results depends on the methods used to assess the age at menarche.

There are three main methods of assessing the age of menstruation: the status quo method (responders answer a question about the occurrence of the first menstruation of girls, most often used at the age of 9–16 years), the retrospective method (responders answer a question of when the first menstruation occurred in girls and women, most often used in participants after 16 years of age and the prospective method (based on a question when the first menstruation occurred, asked during longitudinal studies including girls from about nine years of age. The retrospective method is the most commonly used methods used to estimate the age at menarche. Girls/women are asked to provide the age of the onset of menstruation to the nearest year and month, e.g., I was 12 years and 4 months (12.33 years). It is very rare for a woman to remember the age of her first menstruation to the nearest day. To calculate the average value in the sample of adult women, potentially all study participants are considered for analyses because all of them are post-menarche. The question arises: what size of menarche age memory error can be expected in women asked about it at different stages of life? Could this error be influenced by the woman’s age at the time of the examination, i.e., the temporal distance from the date of menarche? For instance, the longer period of time has passed since the time of menarche, the more the recalled (retrospective) age differs from the actual age at menarche, i.e., prospective one. This problem can only be recognized in longitudinal studies (as in this study), which there are relatively few (Livson and McNeil 1962; Damon et al. 1969; Damon and Bajema 1974; Casey et al. 1991; Must et al. 2002; Żarów and Cichocka 2008). In the available studies, the differences between prospective and retrospective age at menarche ranged from -0.50 to +0.17 years. The size of the differences is significant, which is a subject for further analysis. In continuous studies of a selected group of girls/women from Krakow, which have been ongoing since 1976, the age at menarche was obtained at 3 ontogeny points, including 2 checking long-term memory (retrospective method) in relation to the well-established actual age of menarche (prospective method).

The aim of the present study was to determine whether the date of menarche recreated in women’s memory differs from the actual date determined by prospectively, in adolescence, and what are the magnitude and direction of the memory errors. This study adds to the debate regarding how reliable the retrospective age at menarche is as a measure of changes in the rate of puberty in girls when used to track successive birth cohorts in a sample. This study also aims to determine whether in the study of the inheritance of the rate of puberty between mothers and daughters, the obtained results may be distorted by the age of the individuals at the time of examination.

Material and methods

Research material consisted of data derived from a longitudinal study focused on somatic development and physical fitness of girls and boys, conducted in 1976–2022 by the Department of Anthropology of the Institute of Biomedical Sciences of the University School of Physical Education in Krakow (KBC 1976–2022). The first series of annual surveys was performed in the years 1976–1988 (age of the participants 6–18 years), and the second series in the years 1980–1990; the age of the respondents was 8–19 years. The study analysed data combined from two series examined in 2004 (age 32–34 years) and re-examined in 2022 (age 50–52 years). Sample size and age of the examined girls and women:

Descriptive statistics, such as mean and measures of variability were calculated. Statistical significance of the differences between the analyzed assessment methods was calculated using the Student’s t-test for dependent samples. The analyses were conducted using the Statistica 13.0 software. The numerical statement shows that the number of women in subsequent studies decreased, which is the expected trend in longitudinal studies. In the first and second series of the study, information about the age of the first menstruation was collected using the prospective method. In the studies conducted in 2004 and 2022, a retrospective method was used. Of the 47 women surveyed in 2022, 25% reported secondary education, and the remaining 75% (35 people) stated higher education. All studies were conducted with the consent of the girls’ parents and test subjects. The consent of the Bioethics Committee at the Regional Medical Chamber in Krakow was obtained for the examination in 2022 (consent no. 65/KBL/OIL of April 11, 2022). All procedures contributing to the study complied with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Results

Table 1 shows individual data on age and menstruation according to the prospective and retrospective methods of the surveyed girls and women.

Of 47 women examined in 2022, only four cases in the age of menarche found in the longitudinal study was consisted with the one self-reported by the participants in 2004 and 2022. Ten women declared the same age of menarche while examined in 2004 and 2022 aged 32–34 and 50–52 respectively. In the remaining 33 cases, there was a discrepancy between the age found in the continuous surveys and the age declared in 2004, between the age found in the continuous tests and in 2022, or between the age stated in 2004 and in 2022. Of the 47 women who participated in the 2022 study, 36 also had information about their age of the first menstruation available in the data from continuous studies regarding the reported age of menarche in 2004. For this sample, arithmetic mean, standard deviation and range of the age of menarche were given at three points in women’s ontogeny – as a teenager, at 32–34 and at 50–52 years old (Table 2).

Table 1. The age of the first menstruation data according to the prospective method and the retrospective method – age in years (KBC 1976–2022)
Serial number Test No Continuous tests 1976-1988 and 1978-1990 Research in 2004
Women aged 32-34
Research in 2022
Women aged 50-52
Difference between continuous testing and 2004 Difference between continuous testing and 2022 Difference between 2004 and 2022
Age of first menstruation consistent in all studies
1 768 12 12 12 0 0 0
2 324 13 13 13 0 0 0
3 181 15 15 15 0 0 0
4 410 16 16 16 0 0 0
Age of first menstruation consistent in 2004 and 2022 surveys, and divergent in relation to continuous studies
5 394 11.08 12 12 -0.92 -0.92 0
6 431 11.42 11 11 0.42 0.42 0
7 684 11.75 13 13 -1.25 -1.25 0
8 998 11.92 11 11 0.92 0.92 0
9 757 12.33 12 12 0.33 0.33 0
10 650 12.5 12 12 0.5 0.5 0
11 605 13.42 14 14 -0.58 -0.58 0
12 399 13.75 14 14 -0.25 -0.25 0
13 746 13.92 13.58 13.58 0.33 0.33 0
14 437 14.67 13 13 1.67 1.67 0
Age of first menstruation divergent across all studies
15 888 10.5 10 10.08 0.5 0.42 -0.08
16 918 11.58 11.5 11.58 0.08 0 -0.08
17 60 11.58 . 10.25 1.33
18 237 11.67 12.5 12 -0.83 -0.33 0.5
19 442 12.17 . 15 -2.83
20 871 12.25 12.42 13 -0.17 -0.75 -0.58
21 457 12.33 . 10.25 2.08
22 314 12.5 . 12.25 0.25
23 313 12.58 12.5 11.83 0.08 0.75 0.67
24 785 12.58 11 10.5 1.58 2.08 0.5
25 752 12.83 12.33 14.5 0.5 -1.67 -2.17
26 659 12.83 14 12.92 -1.17 -0.08 1.08
27 214 12.92 16 11 -3.08 1.92 5
28 777 13 14 13.5 -1 -0.5 0.5
29 415 13.08 14 13.92 -0.92 -0.83 -0.08
30 349 13.17 . 13.5 -0.33
31 633 13.33 14 14.75 -0.67 -1.42 -0.75
32 671 13.5 13 17 0.5 -3.5 -4
33 173 13.5 13 14 0.5 -0.5 -1
34 523 13.58 12.67 12.08 -0.92 1.5 0.58
35 629 13.67 13.50 14.17 0.17 0.5 -0.67
36 890 13.75 13.58 12.75 0.16 1 -0.75
37 623 13.75 14 14.92 -0.25 -1.17 -0.92
38 74 13.75 . 12.83 0.92
39 343 13.83 14.08 13.92 -0.25 -0.08 0.17
40 847 13.92 . 14.5 -0.58
41 387 13.92 13 16 0.92 -2.08 -3
42 524 14.08 14 14.67 0.08 -0.58 -0.67
43 790 14.08 . 16.42 -2.33
44 601 14.42 . 14.25 0.18
45 646 14.42 15 16 -0.58 -1.58 -1
46 965 14.58 . 16 -1.42
47 67 14.92 . 16.67 -1.75

. did not participate in the study

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of women’s age of the first menstruation; age is stated in years (KBC 1976–2022, n=36)
Parameter Continuous testing
Prospective method
2004
Women aged 32-34
Retrospective method
2022
Women aged 32-34
Retrospective method
Arithmetic mean 13.05 13.10 13.24
SD 1.15 1.36 1.68
Range 10.50 – 16.00 10.00 – 16.00 10.08 – 17.00

Table 2 shows that the average age of menarche that was self-reported in 2004 was higher by almost one month (0.05 years), and in 2022 – by more than two months (0.19 years) from the corresponding age calculated from the longitudinal data. Thus, older by 18 years women reported a later age of menarche. For the current study group, the value of the Student’s T-test for dependent samples was t=0.9875, p=0.33, the number of degrees of freedom df=35, i.e., a statistically insignificant difference between the data given in found in continuous studies and found in 2022. The differences between the arithmetic mean of the continuous surveys and the mean of the 2004 surveys (t=0.3346, p=0.74, df=35), as well as between the means in 2004 and 2022, were also not statistically significant (t=0.6269, p=0.53, df=35).

Discussion

The age of the first menstruation is influenced by both genetic factors and the external environment. The relationship of the age at menarche with body structure, diet quality, physical activity, personality type, geographical environment, socio-economic status of parents, as well as the impact of psychosocial stimuli has been reported in many studies (e.g., Rees 1995; Gonzales et al. 1996; Tahirović 1998; Chowdhury et al. 2000; Cichocka and Żarów 2002; Barkai et al. 2007; Nieczuja-Dwojacka et al. 2018; Durda-­-Masny et al. 2019; Karim et al. 2021; Glass et al. 2022).

As can be seen from the data presented in table 2, the results obtained using the two methods varied although differences between the data self-reported in 2004 and 2022 were statistically insignificant. These differences would potentially reach a significance level if the number of examined individuals (i.e., sample size) exceeded 140 people (i.e., with the number of n=144, the value of the t statistic=1.996, with p=0.05 [exact value - 0.048]). However, collecting so much data in such long-term continuous studies is extremely difficult. Still, the difference in arithmetic means in 2004 and 2022, amounting to 13.10 and 13.24, respectively, would be statistically significant with the sample exceeding 350 respondents (i.e., with n=360, t-statistic value =2.008 with p=0.05). Such, and larger, samples are regularly reported in comparative cross-sectional studies involving analyzes of secular trends in the age of menarche. The mean age at menarche obtained using the retrospective method was reported as later than the one determined using the prospective method (Table 2), and this difference was smaller if the examination date was closer in time, than the first menstruation. A similar tendency was observed by Koo and Rohan (1997), but the interval after the repeated questions regarding the age of menarche was much shorter – nearly 1 year and almost 2 years. With an average time interval of 323 days, 66.1% of study participants were able to recall the age at menarche correctly, while with an average interval of 649 days, only 44.8% of the subjects were able to do that. In our study, only 11% of women aged 32–34 reported the exact age of menstruation (4 out of 36 women) determined in continuous studies, and at the age of 50–52 years only 10.6% did so (5 out of 47 women) although the average error in the 2022 study was greater.

A greater overestimation of the average age of menarche may be due to a greater memory error of older women. The complicated relationship between the age of the first menstruation and the time of collecting information about this age was the subject of analyses by Mirzaei et al. (2019), who found that the more distant the studies were from the occurrence of menarche, the greater the memory error. At this stage of knowledge about the mechanisms of memory and their changes with age, it is difficult to explain why, over time, on average women continuously perceive their age at menarche as older. This tendency may be reflected in various studies, including analyses of secular changes in the age of menarche. When we examine the retrospective age of menarche (and this is the only age we have at our disposal in studies of adult women) in subsequent birth cohorts of women in the sample, we find the phenomenon of acceleration of puberty, i.e., the younger the years of birth, the earlier the age at menarche. It cannot be ruled out that this pattern of secular changes is caused by the tendency of women’s memory to delay the age of menarche, i.e., the older the age of the respondents, the greater the delay. Estimating the pace of the changes per decade in such a way is subject to error. In our analysis, the average assessment error (overestimation) was 0.19 years, i.e., over 2 months. Kraków is one of the best-researched populations in Poland in terms of determining intergenerational changes in the pace of children’s maturation. Between 1971 and 2010, 4 large series of cross-sectional studies have been conducted monitoring age at menarche using the status quo method (no recall bias). The decrease in the age of puberty in this period was 0.44 years, i.e., an average of 0.11 years per decade, or 1.5 months (Cichocka et al. 2012). The changes were therefore subtle and reliably determined, 1–2 months per decade. The magnitude of the trend per decade was similar to the magnitude of the age at menarche memory error in women at 50–52 years old. A question can be asked – did women in the prospective study tend to underestimate or overestimate their age at menarche compared to the age assessed prospectively? The analysis presented in Table 1 shows that in 2004 (women aged 32–34 years) 15 women underestimated the age of the first menstruation and 17 overestimated it, while in 2022 (women aged 50–52 years) women more often overstated the age of menarche; 19 women underestimated and 12 overestimated it while 1 woman reported the same age. The sample has been reduced by 4 women who reported the same age in all study series. There was also no clear difference in the assessment of the age at menarche between early and late maturing women. Out of these 36, early maturing women were conventionally defined as those whose first menstruation occurred before the age of 12.50 and those whose first menses happened after the age of 13.50 were classified as late maturing. Among the 10 early maturing women, 1 provided the exact age at menarche, 4 overestimated and 5 underestimated the age of menarche. In the group of late maturing women there were 2, 5 and 6 women in the same recall categories, respectively. Longitudinal studies in general, including ours, have weaknesses mainly because they are difficult to implement, time-consuming and expensive, and over time, an increasing rate of loss of participants (dropouts, loss to follow-up) is a common problem. This means that after several decades of collecting longitudinal data, only several dozen participants usually remain from the original sample. For these reasons there are not many such studies. Our research seems to be unique because we have 3 points in ontogeny, temporally distant, examining the age of menarche, including 2 testing long-term memory. An additional advantage of our study is that the research was largely conducted by the same people, which inspired trust among the surveyed women, helping them answer the questions, often involving personal matters, with more confidence and honestly.

Conclusions

  1. The retrospective method that is often used in assessing the age of first menstruation is not fully reliable, as the average discrepancy in assessment ranges from nearly 1 month (0.05 years) to more than 2 months (0.19 years) compared to the prospective method.
  2. Women aged 32–34 years more accurately stated the age of the first menstruation compared to statements obtained from the same women18 years later.
  3. Long-term memory (LTM), even regarding such a significant event in the life of every woman, is often unreliable, as indicated by the difference in the declared age of the first menstruation of the same women surveyed in 2004 and 2022, in some cases amounting to 3, 4 or 5 years.

Overall, in the analysis of secular trends of the age of menarche, it is worth remembering to determine the size of the trend using the same method of assessing the age of menarche. The use of various methods can lead to unreliable results, as we have already previously shown (Żarów and Cichocka 2008; Cichocka et al. 2012), and the magnitudes of the trend can be subtle. As a result, this may cause an erroneous assessment of the conditions of intergenerational changes of the age at menarche.


Acknowledgement
The authors thank all participants for their participation in the research.


Source of financial support
The study was financed within the program of the Ministry of Science and Higher Education in Poland as the ‘Regional Initiative of Excellence’ in the years 2019–2022 (Project No. 022/RID/2018/19) in the amount of 11 919 908 PLN (internal number at University: 35/PB/RID/2022).


Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.


Authors’ contributions
RŻ: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, software, formal analysis, validation, interpretation of the data and results, writing of the original draft, project administration, data curation; AW: investigation, software, writing of the original draft; BS: investigation, formal analysis, validation, interpretation of the data and results, data curation; interpretation of the data; MK: investigation, software, interpretation of the data; JB: investigation, software, interpretation of the data; all authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.



* Corresponding author: Ryszard Żarów, Department of Anthropology, University School of Physical Education of Krakow, Jana Pawła II 78, 31-571 Kraków, Poland, phone: 693741775; e-mail: wazarow@cyf-kr.edu.pl


References

Barkai H-S, Nichols JF, Rauh MJ, Barrack MT, Lawson MJ, Levy SS. 2007. Influence of sports participation and menarche on bone mineral density of female high school athletes. J Sci Med Sport 10(3):170–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2006.05.018

Bergsten-Bruceford A. 1976. A note on the accuracy of recalled age at menarche. Ann Hum Biol 3:71–3. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014467600001151

Brix N, Ernst A, Lauridsen LLB, Parner E, Støvring H, Olsen J, Henriksen TB, Ramlau-Hansen CH. 2019. Timing of puberty in boys and girls: A population-based study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 33(1):70–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12507

Casey VA, Dwyer JT, Coleman KA, Krall EA, Gardner J, Valadian I. 1991. Accuracy of recall by middle-aged participants in a longitudinal study of their body size and indexes of maturation earlier in life. Ann Hum Biol 18:155–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014469100001492

Chowdhury S, Shahabuddin AK, Seal AJ, Talukder KK, Hassan Q, Begum RA, Rahman Q, Tomkins A, Costello A, Talukder MQ. 2000. Nutritional status and age at menarche in a rural area of Bangladesh. Ann Hum Biol 27(3):249–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/030144600282136

Cichocka B, Żarów R. 2002. Secular changes of age at menarche in girls living in Kraków, Warsaw and Wrocław in 1965–2000 and their psychosocial situation (in polish). Pediatrics Poland 77(4):317–322.

Cichocka BA, Woronkowicz A, Kowal M, Sobiecki J, Kryst Ł, Kruszelnicki P, Cichocki S, Kowalska N, Lubecka-Fraszczyńska K, Łukasik M, Piskorz E. 2012. The Ongoing age at menarche acceleration in girls from Cracow (Poland) (in polish). Pediatrics Poland 87(5):460466.

Durda-Masny M, Hanć T, Czapla Z, Szwed A. 2019. BMI at menarche and timing of growth spurt and puberty in Polish girls – longitudinal study. Anthropol Anz 28;76(1):37–47. https://doi.org/10.1127/anthranz/2019/0920

Damon A, Bajema CJ. 1974. Age at menarche: accuracy of recall after thirty-nine years. Hum Biol 46:381–4.

Damon A, Damon ST, Reed RB, Valadian I. 1969. Age at menarche of mothers and daughters, with a note on accuracy of recall. Hum Biol 41(2):160–75.

Glass DJ, Geerkens JT, Martin MA. 2022. Psychosocial and energetic factors on human female pubertal timing: a systematized review. Evol Hum Sci 9;4:e28. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.24

Gomuła A, Koziel S. 2018. Secular trend and social variation in age at menarche among Polish schoolgirls before and after the political transformation. Am J Hum Biol 30(1). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23048

Gonzales GF, Villena A. 1996. Body mass index and age at menarche in Peruvian children living at high altitude and at sea level. Hum Biol 68(2):265–75.

Karim A, Qaisar R, Hussain MA. 2021. Growth and socio-economic status, influence on the age at menarche in school going girls. J Adolesc 86:40–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.12.001

Koo MM, Rohan TE. 1997. Accuracy of short-term recall of age at menarche. Ann Hum Biol 24(1)61–64. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014469700004782

Liu W, Yan X, Li C, Shu Q, Chen M, Cai L, You D. 2021. A secular trend in age at menarche in Yunnan Province, China: a multiethnic population study of 1,275,000 women. BMC Public Health 19;21(1):1890. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11951-x

Livson N, McNeill D. 1962. The accuracy of recalled age at menarche. Hum Biol 34:218–21.

Mirzaei S, Sengupta D, Ghosal R. 2020. Eestimating menarcheal age distribution from partiallyrecalled data. Biostatistics 4(21):876–894. https://doi.org/10.1093/biostatistics/kxz013

Nieczuja-Dwojacka J, Siniarska A, Kozieł S, Marchewka J, Zabłocka R. 2018. Age at maturation, body structure and their relationship with socioeconomic factors. Anthropol Anz 11;75(4):263–270. https://doi.org/10.1127/anthranz/2018/0873

Pop RM, Tenenboum A, Pop M. 2021. Secular Trends in Height, Body Mass and Mean Menarche Age in Romanian Children and Adolescents, 1936–2016. Int J Environ Res Public Health 9;18(2):490. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18020490

Rees M. 1995. The age of menarche. ORGYN (4):2–4.

Tahirović HF. 1998. Menarchal age and the stress of war: an example from Bosnia. Eur J Pediatr 157(12):978–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004310050981

Wu X, Bao L, Du Z, Liu X, Liao W, Kang N, Sun C, Abdulai T, Zhai Z, Wang C, Li Y. 2022. Secular trends of age at menarche and the effect of famine exposure on age at menarche in rural Chinese women. Ann Hum Biol 49(1):35–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460.2022.2041092

Żarów R, Cichocka B. 2008. A comparative analysis of estimation of age at menarche by various methods in women participating in the Krakow Longitudinal Growth Study, Poland. Am J of Hum Biol 20(2):146–148. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20701

COPE

cc
Original article
© by the author, licensee Polish Anthropological Association and University of Lodz, Poland
This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)
Received: 19.10.2023; Revised: 21.11.2023; Accepted: 23.11.2023.